Pre-activation negativity (PrAN)
The pre-activation negativity (PrAN) is an electrically negative brain potential discovered in our lab. It shows the predictive strength of phonological cues. Already at word onset, PrAN has higher amplitude for word beginnings that have few possible word continuations of high lexical frequency (Söderström, Horne, Frid & Roll, 2016; Roll, Söderström, Frid, Mannfolk, & Horne, 2017).
PrAN and cohort size
The number of possible word continuations is also referred to as the word beginning's cohort size (Marslen-Wilson, 1984). Thus, word beginnings with smaller cohort sizes have a larger PrAN (Söderström et al., 2016). PrAN amplitude is also larger if the words of the cohort have high lexical frequency (Roll et al., 2017). Since PrAN is a negative component, its decrease with cohort size is positive and its increase with lexical frequency is negative:
PrAN = k (cohort size) – c (lexical frequency)
where k and c are constants.
Left-edge boundary tones
We have discovered a "left-edge boundary tone" (LEBT) occurring in the first prosodic word of Swedish main clauses but not in subordinate clauses (Roll, 2006; cf. Myrberg, 2010). The complementizer att 'that' can introduce both main clauses and subordinate clauses, which would be temporarily ambiguous were it not for the LEBT. The presence or absence of a LEBT can therefore lead to garden path effects. For example, with no LEBT, listeners expect subordinate clause structure. If they instead hear a word order typical of main clauses, they have been lead down the garden path. Using ERP, reanalysis of the unexpected structure is reflected in a P600 effect (Roll, Horne, & Lindgren, 2009; 2011; Roll & Horne, 2011). Upon hearing that a LEBT is missing at the beginning of a clause, listeners strongly activate subordinate clause structure. This gives rise to a PrAN with its most likely sources in Broca's area (Söderström, Horne, Mannfolk, van Westen, & Roll, 2018).
Dual route processing, word accents, and cortical thickness
Two different routes of processing are assumed to be available for morphologically complex words (Pinker, 1991). Common, frequent forms of words are thought to be stored as whole word forms for quick access, involving Wernicke's area and adjacent regions in the left temporal lobe. Infrequent or previously not encountered forms, on the other hand, can be decomposed into stem and affixes to access the stem meaning. In other words, we can understand an incorrect form like *springde *'runned' by decomposing the word into the stem stem and spring+de 'run+PST.' Decomposition, however, involves the 'dorsal route' for grammatical processing (Friederici, 2017), in particular the posterior portion of Broca's area. This processing is likely to be what is reflected in the ERP effect for misapplication of a regular grammatical suffix on an irregular stem, as in *spring+de *'runn+ed': a left anterior negativity (LAN) (Schremm, Novén, Horne, & Roll, 2019). Suffixes that have been incorrectly cued by the wrong word accent in pseudowords with real suffixes like kvup+ar 'kvup+PL' produce a LAN, indicating that an abstract rule associating tone and suffix might be at play (Söderström, Horne, & Roll, 2017). In fact, just like stem-suffix processing in general, tone-suffix association seems to be represented in two possible routes in the brain. Evidence for dual route representation of word accents comes from correlations between cortical thickness and Swedish speakers' capacity for processing their own native language. Thus, native speakers with thicker cortex in Wernicke's area and the mid and ventral anterior temporal lobe process the word accent-suffix associations quicker in frequent real words than speakers with thinner cortex, suggesting that strong whole form representations are important for real words. Conversely, native speakers with thicker cortex in the posterior portion of Broca's area are quicker to process word accent-suffix association as an abstract rule in pseudowords with real stems (Schremm, Novén, Horne, Söderström, van Westen, & Roll, 2018; Novén, Schremm, Horne, & Roll, 2020).
Processing word meaning
We use words to communicate our thoughts. Words can express different kinds of concepts: specific (SPEC) concrete objects like wrench, more general (GEN) things like tools, more abstract (ABS) concepts like building, and even emotionally (EMO) colored concepts like prison. Using neurolinguistic methods, we have investigated how these different kinds of words are processed and how their meanings are related to various linguistic and cognitive parameters (Blomberg, Roll, Lindgren, Brännström, and Horne, 2015). The ERP N400 component is one way of getting a better understanding of how these different factors interact when we process words and we have found e.g. that the greater the number of discourse related semantic neighbors a word has, the lower its N400 is. General words like tool have relatively many semantic neighbors, whereas more specific words like wrench have relatively fewer such neighbors, and pseudowords (PSEU) have none. Another factor that leads to lowering the N400 is a relatively high level of emotional arousal, whereas a relatively large number of orthographic neighbors has the opposite effect (Blomberg, Roll, Frid, Lindgren, and Horne, 2020).
When the brain suffers a lesion, due e.g. to stroke, the way words are processed can be drastically hampered. We have seen that if the visual cortex receives a lesion, it becomes difficult for speakers to understand concrete words like wrench whose meaning crucially involves being able to process visually related semantic information about shape and color (Mårtensson, Roll, Lindgren, Apt, & Horne, 2014). Abstract words on the other hand can still be understood and produced since they do not rely on being able to process visual meaning components to the same degree (Roll, Mårtensson, Sikström, Apt, Arnling-Bååth, & Horne, 2012).